Infancy ch. 8 - The Development of Language

phonology

the sound patterns of a particular language and the rules for combining them

semantics

a particular language's system of meaning and the rules for conveying/representing meaning

syntax

the rules for combining words into sentences in a particular language

prelinguistic phase

the period before a child speaks his or her first words in which language development has already begun

birth-1 month

cry: the predominant sound (also fussing, gurgling, and satisfied sounds)

1-2 months

laughing and cooing

6-7 months

-babbling makes up about half of babies' noncrying sounds from about 6-12 months and rapidly increases; begins as including all kinds of sounds, even some not part of the language they are hearing
-delightful and easy for adults to imitate
-gradually deve

9-10 months

-babbling shifts toward set of sounds listened to with non-heard sounds dropping out
-May reflect synaptic pruning
-Gestural language develops (i.e., pointing) - first signs of expressive language

expressive language

sounds, signs, or symbols used to communicate meaning (pointing is most common among infants; gestures and sounds may also be combined)

cooing

making repetitive vowel sounds, particularly the uuu sound; develops early in the prelinguistic period, when babies are between about 1-4 months

babbling

the repetitive vocalizing of consonant-vowel combinations by an infant, typically beginning around 6 months

receptive language

comprehension of spoken language; first signs become evident around 9 or 10 months

12-13 months

1st words typically appear

definition of a "word

for a child, it can be any sound; doesn't have to be a sound that matches words adults are using

16 months

first vocabulary spurt; speaking vocabulary of 50 words; usually names for things or people

24 months

320 words; generalize new words to many situations; verbs develop slowly

underextension

use of a word for only one specific object or in a single context; most common at the earliest stages of vocabulary development, particularly before the naming explosion, which suggests that most children initially think of words as belonging to only one

overextension

the inappropriate use of a single word for a category of objects or multiple contexts; more common during naming explosion; may reflect lack of vocabulary more than inability to discriminate

fast-mapping

ability to categorically link new words to real-world referents; evident as early as 18 months, but a momentous shift in the way children approach new words happens around age 3, and they begin to pay attention to words in whole groups, and this really ap

constraint

an assumption or bias that is presumed to be built-in or learned early (a "default option") by which a child figures out what words refer to

whole object constraint

the assumption that words refer to whole objects and not to their parts or attributes.

mutual exclusivity constraint

leads children to assume that objects have only one name

principle of contrast

the assumption that every word has a different meaning, which leads a child to assume that two or more different words refer to different objects

12-18 months

first holophrases

18-24 months

f8rst tw9-word sentences

holophrase

a combination of gesture and a single word that conveys more meaning than just the word alone; often seen and heard in children between 12-18 months old

telegraphic speech

term used by Roger Brown to describe the earliest sentences created by most children, which sound a bit like telegrams because they include key nouns and verbs but generally omit all other words and grammatical inflections; also called stage 1 grammar, an

parts of speech first used

nouns, verbs, and adjectives (more nouns are typically learned more quickly)

adding inflections

not added all at once; within each language community, children seem to add these and more complex word orders in fairly predictable sequences. In English, the earliest is usually adding -ing to a verb, then perpositions (on and in), plural -s on nouns, i

questions

child puts a wh word at the beginning of a sentence, but doesn't put the auxiliary verb in the right place; rather quickly figures out correct forms

negatives

child uses no or not without proper auxiliary verb placement; rather quickly figures out correct forms and stops making these mistakes

overregularization

young children's applications of basic rules to irregular words; they initially learn a small number of irregular words (past tenses) and use them correctly for a short time; then they learn to add -ed to the end of words and overgeneralize this rule to a

complex sentences

follow inflections and negation and allow children to use conjunctions to connect two or more ideas; major strides taken during 3-4 years

pragmatics

the rules for the use of language in communicative interaction, such as the rules for taking turns and the style of speech that is appropriate for different listeners; the way in which children learn to use language/speech either to communicate or to regu

Pragmatic abilities at different ages

-18 mos. - patterns of gazing at 18 months similar to adults.
-2 yrs. - adapt the form of language to the situation they are in or the person to whom they are talking.
-4 yrs. - begin to adapt language to improve communication with their intended audience

Private speech

language used by children to help control or monitor their behavior; may consist of fragmentary sentences, muttering, or instructions to themselves, and is detectable from the earliest use of words and sentence. Vygotsky argues that this help children com

Motherese (infant-directed speech)

the simplified, higher-pitched speech that adults use with infants and young children. Adults repeat often, introduce minor variations, and use slightly more elongated sentences than the child. A baby more easily imitates a correct grammatical form that a

Chomsky

an early nativist theorist who suggests learning language is an innate ability; was especially struck by 2 phenomena: the extreme complexity of the task the child must accomplish, and the apparent similarities in the steps and stages of children's early l

MLU (mean length of utterance)

the average number of meaningful units in a sentence. Each word is one meaningful unit, as is each inflection. Frequently used by physicians, teachers, and others to identify children who need additional screening to determine whether they have some kind

phonological awareness

understanding of the rules governing the sounds of a language as well as knowledge of the connection between sounds and the way they are represented in written language. Children who are more phonologically aware at age 3, 4, and 5 learn to read much more

invented spelling

a strategy young children with good phonological awareness skills use when they write; despite errors, children who use this before receiving formal instruction in reading and writing are more likely to become good spellers and readers later in childhood.

whole language approach

an approach to reading instruction that places more emphasis on the meaning of written language than on its structure; says that most children are capable of inferring letter-sound correspondence on their own as long as they have enough exposure to print.

balanced approach

reading instruction that combines explicit phonics instruction with other strategies for helping children acquire literacy

systematic and explicit phonics

planned, specific instruction in sound-letter correspondences

becoming literate in school

later reading success is promoted by learning about meaningful word parts (prefixes and suffixes) and instruction in comprehension strategies (identifying the purpose of a particular text)

Poor readers

have problems with sound-letter combinations; curriculum flexibility is needed; without enrichment, poor readers do not catch up to their peers

English-language learners (ELLs)

school children who do not speak English well enough to function in English-only classes

bilingual education

as practiced in the U.S., a school program for students who are not proficient in English in which instruction in basic subject matter is given in the children's native language during the first 2 or 3 years of schooling, with a gradual transition to full

structured immersion

an alternative to traditional bilingual education used in classrooms in which all children speak the same non-English native language. All basic instruction is in English, paced so that the children can comprehend, with the teacher translating only when a

English-as-a-second-language (ESL)

an alternative to bilingual education; children who are not proficient in English attend academic classes taught entirely in English but then spend several hours in a separate class to receive English-language instruction; most common in U.S.

submersion

an approach to education of non-English-speaking students in which they are assigned to a classroom where instruction is given in English and are given no supplemental language assistance; aka "sink or swim" approach